The Semiotic Nature of Number Knowledge: Contributions to the Educational Field

Authors

  • Jimena Soledad Rodríguez IRICE, CONICET.
  • Eduard Martí Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Barcelona.
  • Analia Salsa IRICE, CONICET.

Keywords:

number, quantity, semiotic systems, concrete materials

Abstract

In this article we argue that semiotic systems (gestural, linguistic, graphic signs) are not only different supporting tools of number but, as constitutive elements of our cognition, they act as nuclear organizers of number knowledge. First, we review the Piagetian and neonativist perspectives in order to reveal the subsidiary role that they give to semiotic components in the acquisition of number. Second, we identify and analyse the particularities and cognitive complexity of different kinds of number representations in order to, finally, provide a reflexion regarding the use of pictures and symbolic objects as concrete materials for mathematics teaching to young children.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biography

Analia Salsa, IRICE, CONICET.

Dra. en Psicología.

References

Bialystok, E., & Cood, J. (1996). Developing representations of quantity. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 28, 281-291.

Brunner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Belkapp Press.

Cavalcante, S., & Rodríguez, C. (2015). The understanding of die as an object that has numerical functions. A longitudinal study using two children from the ages of 24 to 36 months interacting with an adult. Estudios de Psicología, 36, 48-70.

Condry, K., & Spelke, E. (2008). The development of language and abstract concepts: The case of natural number. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 137, 22-38.

Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Pica, P., & Spelke, E. (2006). Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, 311, 381-384.

DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238, 1556-1557.

DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic Functioning in Very Young Children: Understanding of Pictures and Models. Child Development, 62, 736–752.

DeLoache, J. S. (1995). Early understanding and use of symbols: The model model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 109-113.

DeLoache, J. S. (2002). Symbolic artifacts: Understanding and use. En U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 206-226). London: Blackwell Publishing.

García-Mila, M., Andersen, C., & Rojo, N. (2010). Pensar e “inscribir” mientras se investiga: un estudio de caso. Cultura y Educación, 22, 199-213.

Geary, D. C. (2006). Development of mathematical understanding. En D. Kuhl, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Volume 2. Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 777-810). New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. (1978). The child's understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gunderson, E. A., & Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than others: relations between parents' input and children's cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science, 14, 1021-1032.

Harris, R. (1995). Signs of writing. New York, NY: Routledge.

Hirschfeld, L., & Gelman, S. (1994). Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Huang, Y., Spelke, E., & Snedeker, J. (2010). When is four far more than three? Children's generalization of newly-acquired number words. Psychological Science, 21, 600-606.

Kamii, C. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget's theory. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G., Brannon, E., & Carey, S. (2006). Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 130–169.

Levine, S., Suriyakham, L., Rowe, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Gunderson, E. (2010). What Counts in the Development of Young Children's Number Knowledge? Developmental Psychology, 46, 1309–1319.

Martí, E. (2003). Representar el mundo externamente. La adquisición infantil de los sistemas externos de representación. Madrid: Machado.

Martí, E. (2012). Desarrollo del pensamiento e instrumentos culturales. En M. Carretero, & J. A. Castorina (Eds.), Desarrollo cognitivo y educación (II). Procesos y contenidos específicos (pp. 77-100). Buenos Aires: Paidós.

Martí, E., & García-Mila, M. (2010). Progresos en la diferenciación funcional entre dibujo, escritura y numerales en niños de 4 a 7 años. Estudios de Psicología, 31, 339-352.

Martí, E., & Pozo, J. I. (2000). Más allá de las representaciones mentales: la adquisición de los sistemas externos de representación. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 23, 11-30.

Martí, E., Scheuer, N., & de la Cruz, M. (2013). Symbolic use of quantitative representations in young children. En B. Brizuela & M.

Gravel (Eds.), Show me what you know. Exploring representations across STEM disciplines. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Mix, K., Sandhofer, C., & Baroody, A. (2005). Number words and number concepts. The interplay of verbal and nonverbal quantification in early childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 33, 305-

Mix, K., Sandhofer, C., Moore, J., & Russell, C. (2012). Acquisition of the cardinal word principle: The role of input. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 274– 283.

Montessori, M. (1917). The advanced Montessori method. Nueva York, NY: Frederick A. Stokes.

Nicoladis, E., Pika, S., & Marentette, P. (2010). Are number gestures easier than number words for preschoolers? Cognitive Development, 25, 247-261.

Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (1996). Children doing mathematics. Oxford: Blackwell.

Nunes, T. (1997). Systems of signs and mathematical reasoning. En T. Nunes, & P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning and teaching mathematics: An international perspective (pp. 29-44). Hove: Psychology Press.

Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper. The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Pérez-Echeverría. M. P., & Scheuer, N. (2005). Desde el sentido numérico al número con sentido. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 28, 393-407.

Petersen, L., & McNeil, N. (2012). Effects of perceptually rich manipulatives on preschooler's counting performance: Established knowledge counts. Child Development, 84, 1020-1033.

Piaget, J. (1970). Science of education and the psychology of the child. New York, NY: Orion Press.

Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1966). La psychologie de l'enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Piaget, J., & Szeminska, A. (1941). La genèse du nombre chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel/Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.

Pimm, D. (1987). Speaking mathematically: Communication in mathematics classrooms. London: Routledge.

Pozo, J. I. (2008). Aprendices y maestros. La psicología cognitiva del aprendizaje.Madrid: Alianza.

Rodríguez, C., & Scheuer, N. (2015). The paradox between the numerically competent baby and the slow learning of two- to four-yearold children. Estudios de Psicología, 36, 18-47.

Salsa, A., & Peralta, O. (2000). Implicancias didácticas del uso y la comprensión temprana de símbolos. Cultura y Educación, 19, 35-45.

Salsa, A., & Martí, E. (2015). Objects, pictures and words. Effects of representational format on four-year-olds' quantity knowledge. Estudios de Psicología, 36, 81–91.

Sarnecka, B., & Carey, S. (2008). How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition, 108, 662-

Sarnecka, B., & Lee, M. (2009). Levels of number knowledge during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 325-

Saxe, G. (1982). The development of measurement operations among the Oksapmin of Papua New Guinea. Child Development, 53, 1242-1248.

Saxe, G. (1991). Culture and cognitive development. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Ass.

Saxe, G., Guberman, S., & Gearhart, M. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52, 1-162.

Scheuer, N., Bressan, A. & Merlo de Rivas, S. (2001). Los conocimientos numéricos en niños que inician su escolaridad. En Nora Elichiry (Comp.)¿Dónde y cómo se aprende? Temas en Psicología Educacional (pp. 99-122). Buenos Aires: Eudeba-JVE.

Sinclair, A. (1991). Children's production and comprehension of written numerical representations. En K. Durkin, & B. Shire (Eds.), Language in mathematical education (pp. 59-68). Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.

Spelke, E. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 1233-1243.Teubal, E., & Guberman, A. (2014). Textos gráficos y alfabetización múltiple. Herramientas para el desarrollo del pensamiento y el aprendizaje en el nivel inicial. Buenos Aires: Paidós.

Tolchinsky, L. (2003). The cradle of culture and what children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Uttal, D., Scudder, K., & DeLoache, J. S. (1997). Manipulatives as symbols: A new perspective on the use of concrete to teach mathematics. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 37-54.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. New York, NY: Wiley.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wiese, H. (2003). Iconic and non-iconic stages in number development: The role of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 385-390.

Wiese, H (2007). The co-evolution of number concepts and counting words. Lingua, 117, 758–772.

Wynn, K. (1990). Children's understanding ofcounting. Cognition, 36, 155-193.

Wynn, K. (1992). Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220-251.

Wynn, K. (1997). Competence models of numerical development. Cognitive Development, 12, 333-339.

Published

02/07/2019

How to Cite

Rodríguez, J. S., Martí, E., & Salsa, A. (2019). The Semiotic Nature of Number Knowledge: Contributions to the Educational Field. Revista De Psicología, 12(23), 67–79. Retrieved from https://erevistas.uca.edu.ar/index.php/RPSI/article/view/1538

Issue

Section

Articles